Skip to main content

Daily rhythm of nociception in rats

Abstract

Background

Many behavioral and physiological variables exhibit daily rhythmicity. Few investigations of the daily rhythmicity in nociception have been conducted, and conflicting results have been obtained. The present study evaluated the daily rhythmicity in nociception in Wistar rats.

Methods

Nociception was investigated by Eddy's hot plate method, tail immersion method, and tail clip method. The latency between the noxious stimulus and the animal's response was recorded as reaction time. Separate groups of rats were tested in 4-hour intervals for 24 hours.

Results

There was clear daily variation in response latency. Reaction time was shortest a few hours before lights-on and longest at the light-dark transition.

Conclusion

Nociception exhibits robust daily rhythmicity in rats. Sensitivity to pain is highest late in the dark phase of the light-dark cycle and lowest at the light-dark transition.

Background

Daily rhythmicity is an ubiquitous property of the physiology and behavior of animals [1]. Understanding of the daily rhythmicity in nociception is important for the standardization of studies of analgesic drugs. Yet, few studies have investigated the daily rhythmicity in nociception. Although studies on rats [2] and golden hamsters [3] have indicated the occurrence of greater pain sensitivity during the dark phase of the light-dark cycle, another study on rats indicated the occurrence of greater sensitivity during the light phase [4], and a study on mice indicated the occurrence of two daily peaks in sensitivity, one during the light phase and one during the dark phase [5]. Therefore, a re-evaluation of the daily rhythmicity in nociception seemed warranted.

Methods

Male albino Wistar rats were purchased from the Chellamuthu Trust, Madurai. They were housed in microlon cages maintained at 25 ± 1°C under an L12:D12 light-dark cycle.

Nociception was evaluated by Eddy's hot plate method, tail immersion method, and tail clip method. The latency between the noxious stimulus and the animal's response was recorded as reaction time. Rats previously adapted to an L12:D12 light-dark cycle were divided into 7 groups of 6 animals and tested at one of 7 times of day 4 hours apart. The same groups of animals were retested a week later with the same protocol, except that the animals initially tested first during the light phase of the light-dark cycle were tested first during the dark phase, and vice versa.

Results and discussion

The results are shown in Fig. 1. Reaction time (average of the three methods) was longest at the transition from light to darkness and shortest a few hours before the transition from darkness to light. This suggests greater pain sensitivity late in the dark phase, which is in agreement with previous studies in rats and golden hamsters [2, 3]. Another study in rats suggested the occurrence of greater sensitivity during the light phase [4], but this was probably an artifact of the experimental procedure, as only two time points during the day were reported. A study on mice suggested the occurrence of two daily peaks in sensitivity, one during the light phase and one during the dark phase [5]. The amplitude of the daily variation in latencies was much smaller in that study than in ours, and it is possible that random oscillations were interpreted as a daily rhythm. Alternatively, species differences may account for the difference in the results.

Figure 1
figure 1_4

Daily rhythmicity of nociception in rats The figure shows the daily variation in reaction time to nociceptive stimulation. Each data point corresponds to the mean (±SE) of 6 rats. The horizontal bar at the top indicates the timing of the light-dark cycle.

Conclusion

It is concluded that nociception exhibits robust daily rhythmicity in rats. Sensitivity to pain is highest late in the dark phase of the light-dark cycle and lowest at the light-dark transition.

References

  1. Takahashi JS, Turek FW, Moore RY: Circadian Clocks. New York, Kluwer/Plenum 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Martinez-Gomez M, Cruz Y, Salas M, Hudson R, Pacheco P: Assessing pain threshold in the rat: changes with estrus and time of day. Physiol Behav 1994, 55:651–657.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Pickard GE: Circadian rhythm of nociception in the golden hamster. Brain Res 1987, 425:395–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rosenfeld JP, Rice PE: Diurnal rhythms in nociceptive thresholds of rats. Physiol Behav 1979, 23:419–420.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Konecka AM, Sroczynska I: Circadian rhythm of pain in male mice. Gen Pharmacol 1998, 31:809–810.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

The authors thank Prof. M. Nagarajan for the encouragement throughout the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to AJM Christina.

Additional information

Competing interests

We, the authors declare that we have not received funds from any agency or organization for carrying out this work.

Authors' contributions

AJMC – Designed the study

NJM – Carried out the study

CV – Carried out the replicate study

NM and SJP – Evaluated the data statistically

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Christina, A., Merlin, N., Vijaya, C. et al. Daily rhythm of nociception in rats. J Circad Rhythms 2, 2 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-2-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-2-2